I.B - Small inventory and classification of colors

The perceived colors are therefore mixtures of monochromatic* light rays. Let’s see how to classify all these colors and how they are decomposed.

Note

We are still talking here about light and light rays, and thus about an additive system; it is not a question of the composition of surfaces and the way they absorb and reflect colors, and which would be the object of a subtractive synthesis of light.
See chapter D - Color Reproduction for more details on the subject.

B.1 - Black

The simplest color is therefore black* : the total absence of light is perceived as black. When the intensity of the light decreases, the colors approach gradually the black. But when the intensity rises, the colors do not go towards white! The higher the intensity, the more the color appears “bright” and “saturated”, but does not fade, does not whiten 1.

B.2 - Monochromatic lights

The simplest colors, and the rarest (they are those emitted by Lasers for example), are those composed of a single monochromatic light. They are in fact the colors of the rainbow, those which are included in the monochromatic spectrum.

Light spectrum

They range from red to blue, orange, yellow, green, cyan… but do not include any shades of purple*.

Monochromatic colors

They can vary in intensity, in a scale from black (intensity 0) to the “brightest” color.

B.3 - Purple

It should be noted that the purple/violet range2 are not part of the “natural” and monochromatic colors but are the result of the mixing of blue and red rays, which are the two extremes of the spectrum.

Purple

The purples added to the monochromatic colors form the set of the most saturated* colors possible, which are thus all monochromatic colors and mixtures containing only blue and red.

Color wheel, with monochromatic/bichromatic separation

B.4 - White and grey

In nature, all the lights, or more precisely all the luminous rays, are monochromatic*; it is the perception of their blending that the brain interprets in an infinity of other colors. We saw that the purples are part of these mixtures; all the other colors not saturated*, the grays and the whites, are thus also mixed lights.

All the “desaturated” shades, whites, grays, are blends of these monochromatic colors, and there is for each color (and not only the grays, all the non-monochromatic colors) an infinity of different blends which can generate it.

Some broken down whites

Two colors perceived in an identical way but composed of monochromatic rays in different proportions are said metamers*.

B.4.a - Complementary colors

A minimum of two “opposite” rays is needed to form white: two monochromatic rays that form grey/white when mixed are said to be complementary*.

We can also include purples as complementary to greens. Indeed, a purple in turn mixed with green gives white3, and can therefore be considered as the complementary of green4, although it is actually already the combination of red and blue. We find there the three common primaries of digital color reproduction systems.

Color wheels with some straight lines representing complementaries

B.4.b - Perception

The same white sheet of paper will be seen as white regardless of the time of day or the type of lighting, although in reality its color is quite different in each of these cases: the sheet of paper will in fact take on the color of the light that illuminates it, but the brain will operate a “shift” in perception that will make it appear white in all cases.

Photographs of snowy landscape at different times of the day
Under the sun, in the shade or in the clouds, whatever the time of day, in reality we perceive snow as white even if in reality the light it reflects is each time different5.

Therefore, rays of light with perfectly different physical attributes can be seen in the same way by the observer. ***The perception of colors is eminently subjective.


META


  1. In the real world. But artistically, the technical limits of color reproduction systems impose to “cheat” by whitening the highlights to compensate the limit of luminosity of the available colors. Without this trick, “monochromatic” colors in an image could appear less luminous than gray or white of the same image; lights are therefore “pulled” towards white values to compensate. 

  2. The term “ultraviolet” to describe the part of the electromagnetic spectrum beyond blue is misleading; the end of the spectrum is perceived as blue (dark) rather than violet. The true violet is the color that we perceive as a result of the combination of blue and red rays. 

  3. We are of course speaking here of light, in other words an “additive” system where the intensities add up. This is obviously not true in painting and printing which are “subtractive” systems where the colors “asborbate” light intensity, subtracting rays of light. cf. chapter D - Reproduction of colors

  4. If we wanted to be physically correct, the greens would simply have no complements at all, and only the intervals of rays near the two extremes of the spectrum (oranges-reds and cyan-blues) would be complementary to each other. 

  5. What a camera has the greatest difficulty to reproduce. see chapter E - About white values : temperature